Tip:
Highlight text to annotate it
X
Developmental dyslexia is a clinical condition characterized by reduced lexical performance
as a function of the age. It is a worldwide problem, found in countries
with very different languages and graphical systems. Its prevalence ranges from 1% in
Egypt up to 20-25% in Brazil. In general its average prevalence in the world is about 3-5 %.
Its aetiologi is not yet clear. However it
is very likely the eredofamiliar component plays an important role.
Undoubtedly, developmental dyslexia is a neuropsychiatric and logopedic condition. Still, even if the
visuoperceptive aspect has been considered less important to date, signs and symptoms
as well as a growing body of evidence suggest the occurrence of subtle alterations involving
the visual domain in a subgroup of patients. We will name these patients as "visual dyslexics".
This finding is predictable, considering that when reading letters have to be correctly
perceived and processed before their conversion into phonemes takes place. Clearly, the perception
and processing of letters is provided by the visual channel.
Probably the reason for why the visual approach has been under-estimated so far, is that during
an ordinary ophthalmological and orthoptic examination dyslexic children turn out to
be normal. Besides, it is restrictive to believe that
just for this reason the patient does not conceal slight visuoperceptive alterations:
in fact, they are not detectable as such by using the ordinary clinical approach but could
be revealed by specific psychophysical techniques. Really, the neurophtalmological alterations
as reported in literature refers both to the oculomotor and sensory domain.
A strand of research maintains that the visual dyslexics suffer from instability of motor
dominance, so as to make imprecise and unstable the fixation of letters and syllables during
their sequential analysis: generally the visual axis of the dominant eye is taken as the reference
one and if a minimal and temporary discrepancy takes place (as often happens), the brain
ignore the non-dominant eye and considers the fixation of the dominant eye to process
the stimulus. On the contrary, if the motor dominance is
unstable, the temporary difference between the visual axes direction of the two eyes
gives a conflictual perceptive situation. This conflict would lead to unstable fixation,
moving to and from around the letter, making reading slow and imprecise.
The same conflict can occur also in case of weak sensory dominance. Subjects who show
stable sensory dominance have chosen their left or right eye as the reference one; in
this case if two different letters fall on the two retinae, the visual system automatically
tends to process the stimulus projected onto the retina of the reference eye. In this way
time after time the letters' processing is made univocal.
Now, if sensory dominance is not stable, when two different letters are up to be processed
simultaneously, a stalemate takes place because the visual system is unable to decide which
of the two stimuli to be processed. In this case, again, slow and inaccurate reading is
expected. So far, the test used for the assessment of
the ocular dominance in dyslexic children is the Dunlop test. Indeed, the Dunlop test
does not seems to be suitable: in fact it is not practical, as it requires a synoptophore,
special slides and a training period for the technician as well as for the patient. In
addition, results are argued to be affected by the age of the child. Finally, the Dunlop
test is unable to split motor and sensory dominance.
A test of dominance practical, fast, easy to use and able to measure separately motor
and sensory dominance is therefore desirable. The advanced solution is the domitest M and
the domitest S. A second stream of research has highlighted
in a subgroup of dyslexics subtle visuoperceptive alterations involving one of the two visual
pathways: the magnocellular system. The magnocellular system is in charge of motion
perception and of contrast sensitivity at high temporal frequencies and low spatial
frequencies. Indeed, contrast sensitivity studies have
found a magnocellular impairment in a consistent proportion of dyslexics. As a matter of fact,
a class of patients seems to be less sensitive to contrast of gratings of spatial frequency
below 1.5 cycles/degree and temporal frequency higher than 10 hertz. If on the one hand the
way this impairment affects reading is unclear, on the other hand such a contrast sensitivity
deficit can be regarded as an epiphenomenon, a marker of visual dyslexics.
It is desirable therefore a test specifically devised to measure contrast sensitivity at
the very low spatial frequencies, found to be defective in dyslexic patients. The exam
available in the Tetra platform satisfies this need: it measures contrast sensitivity
at spatial frequencies of 0.5, 0.75, 1.5 till to 12 cycles/degree. Moreover, it is driven
by a staircase-type alghorithm which makes the contrast sensitivity assessment very fast,
taking less than two minutes. An interesting subject, also under a rehabilitative
point of view, is the finding of increased lateral masking (also known as crowding) in
many dyslexic patients. Normally, adjoining letters making up a word
tend to influence (or mask) each other, so that their perception is made more difficult
compared to the same characters presented separately. The closer are the letters, the
stronger is the lateral masking. Beyond a given spatial interval (called critical spacing)
the reciprocal masking ceases. Now, in many dyslexics the critical spacing
is found to be wider. For this reason adjoining letters that normally do not mask each other,
in patients are crowded. As a matter of fact, widening the interval between characters often
improves the lexical function in disabled readers.
It is showed that abnormal crowding (that is to say wider critical spacing) may depend
on abnormal spatial relationship perception. According to this hypothesis, in dyslexics
the visual space would be "shrunk" along the horizontal axis and "stretched" along the
vertical one. We define this condition "vertical anisotropy".
Presumably, as a perceptive consequence of this abnormality, adjoining letters are illusory
perceived closer, that is to say the critical spacing turns out to be larger.
The estimate of the spatial relationship perception and related anisotropy would be therefore
an important tool for correctly evaluate the visuoperceptive impairment, in each single
patient. For this purpose a specific psychophysical
test aimed at evaluating spatial relationship perception and the related vertical anisotropy
has been devised: the eidomorphometry. Finally, in each patient it is desirable to
assess how and to what extent the alleged visuomotor and visuosensory alteration affect
the lexical task. This is the aim of the reading performance
test or REPORT, suitable to measure the reading rate for words and non-words as a function
of the "stretching" of the lexical string. A complete analysis is performed and the outcome
is presented as indexes and graphs. In conclusion: domitest M, domitest S, Eidomorphometry
and REPORT: complemented each other in the TETRA platform are the solution for those
who intend to study the lexical function under the visuoperceptive perspective.